The strategic approach of the mental research institute
THE MYTH OF THE MAGIC PILL BARRY DUNCAN, PSY.D., SCOTT MILLER, PH.D., AND JACQUELINE SPARKS, MS The Myth of the Magic Pill Barry Duncan, Scott Miller, and Jacqueline Sparks
Shelley fights back the tears as she utters in a voice barely audible to her family physician, “things
have just gotten to be too much—at first I didn’t want to get up for work and now I don’t want to get up at
all.” Shelley feels hopeless, desperate, and fragile. In times gone by, she rallied during tough times and met
obstacles head on. But this time is different; no last minute heroics or rising to the occasion, no platitudes
or pep talks that have always worked for her in the past. The doctor looks at the 10-item questionnaire
Shelley completed in the waiting room; it accurately reflects her poor appetite and loss of pleasure in sex or
anything else for that matter. Her physician explains that such “neurovegetative symptoms” are a sign of
“clinical depression,”a medical illness resulting from a chemical imbalance in the brain. She also tells
Shelley that while the problem is serious, there is no need to despair because the illness is now treatable.
The doctor gives Shelley a brochure that says treating depression is just like using insulin for diabetes. By
taking an antidepressant, Shelley will increase a critical chemical that is in short supply in the brain. As the
examination nears completion, the physician suggests that Shelley seek out the support of a “talk therapist”
as combining medication with psychotherapy can sometimes enhance the chances of recovery. She nods
her agreement. Prescription in hand and samples in her purse, Shelley leaves the office feeling comforted:
her problem has a name, a cause, and a known cure.
The practice of attributing emotional suffering to chemical imbalances in the brain is now
commonplace and embraced by the public and mental health professionals alike. So popular is it, in fact,
that since the antidepressant Prozac--the first SSRI or selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor--was
introduced in 1988, over 300 million prescriptions have been written for the drug and its two chemical
cousins, Paxil and Zoloft. Pharmacological treatment is not only popular for adults but also the fastest
growing form of intervention for children. In 1996, 600,000 prescriptions for Prozac alone were written for
kids under the age of 18 and 203,000 for children between the ages of 6 and 12. Three thousands scripts
were written for infants under the age of one!
So ubiquitous have these drugs become that almost everyone has either taken or knows someone
who is taking medication and reporting feeling better as a result. Sure, the pharmaceutical companies can
be criticized for excessive promotion and marketing. Learning that Eli Lilly long ago created a peppermint
flavored version of Prozac even though it is not approved for use with children and even requires a warning
label (“safety and effectiveness in pediatric patients has not been established”) is enough to make most
people temporarily uncomfortable. By now though, most Americans have come to expect occasional
excesses from businesses competing for market share in the capitalistic society in which we live. And while
most mental health professionals would acknowledge that the explanation given to clients is a gross
oversimplification of actual brain functioning, few reject the biochemical model altogether. Fewer still
question the effectiveness of the drugs, and virtually no one challenges the idea that combining medication
with therapy is the best of all treatment options. At least it includes what talk therapists have to offer. The
problem with these common beliefs and practices emerges, however, when they are examined in the light
of scientific research. Empirically, there is little support for:
the idea that emotional suffering is caused by a biochemical imbalance in the brain;
the superiority of drug treatment over psychotherapy (even for “severe” depression); or
better outcomes when therapy is combined with drugs.
Let’s start with the mantra of American psychiatry: the biochemical imbalance. What our culture
calls “depression” is a complex condition of mind, body, life, and heart. Yet, in the past 10 years, we have
been subjected to a simplistic version of a medieval morality play extensively (and expensively) promoted
by pharmaceutical companies and rarely critically examined. Replayed endlessly in magazine ads, radio
announcements and television shows, endorsed by federal agencies and mental health organizations, this
drama always shows some poor soul possessed by a “Chemical Imbalance Demon” being exorcised by a
high priest with a prescription pad. Never mind the fact that standard medical textbooks say there is no such
thing as a simple “biochemical imbalance” which accounts for emotional problems. Indeed, as neuroscientist
Elliot Valenstein points out in his excellent book, Blaming the Brain, the arguments supporting biochemical
imbalances are unconvincing and the research rudimentary at best. Valenstein suggests that psychotropic drugs
create, not cure, biochemical problems because of the brain’s plasticity and rapid adaptation to pharmaceuticals.
And yet, the message continues to be broadcast to the American public: emotional suffering is a medical
disease and one should run, not walk, to the nearest Church of Pharmacology for a cure. “Biochemical
imbalance” is as much as part of American vernacular as “Did somebody say McDonalds?”
Consider, for example, the following excerpt from a 60 minutes documentary that aired in 1993 at
the beginning of the SSRI’s meteoric rise to power:
For 10 years, D has been suffering from depression, a serious illness. Sometimes she spends
weeks on an unmade bed in a filthy apartment. She told us that she didn’t care about anything, and
I’ve sometimes been afraid to take the subway home because I might throw myself in.
Most doctors believe chronic depression like D’s is caused by a chemical imbalance in the brain.
To correct it, the doctor prescribed Prozac.
. . . and two weeks later, we paid her another visit. I can’t get over it. You’re smiling.
Great. I feel great. I feel like--like a different person. Somebody else. Somebody--something left
She no longer spends her days in a filthy apartment. So two weeks after you started on this drug,
. . . fixed your apartment, fixed yourself . . .
Yeah. Mmm, hmm. Yep. I’m happy about it. I think it’s great.
In this narrative, depression is not a bundle of miseries shaped by many forces, weak and strong: a
sedentary, lonely, dishonest, impoverished or selfish life; perhaps the loss of love, health, beauty or
community; feelings of powerlessness arising from unsatisfying work, oppressive socioeconomic factors,
addicted children or a difficult relationship; frustrated ambitions; difficulties in knowing, speaking and
meeting one’s needs or in getting along with one’s family or other people. No, D on 60 minutes is suffering
from a purely biological illness, a “chemical imbalance.” Its resolution does not require her to get
meaningful support from others, to establish a collaborative relationship with a good psychotherapist, to
change her attitudes and actions or to make any personal effort. There is only one solution needed, and only
one offered: the passive consumption of a pill.
For decades, pharmaceutical companies and their handmaidens have spent billions of dollars promoting
this simplistic message, and it has only intensified since the introduction of the “miracle SSRIs.”
Trumpeting these drugs’ supposedly vast advantage over earlier antidepressants and therapy, drug company
representatives rent booths at psychiatrists’ conventions; buy advertising in medical journals; hand out
manufacturers samples to M.D.’s; talk to journalists; and fund the seemingly incontrovertible drug research
that provides the intellectual undergirding for this miracle play. More recently, the pharmaceutical industry
has bypassed these traditional “middle men of mental health” and marketed their wares directly to the
general public. Antidepressants are now as normal and pervasive as aspirin--like VISA, it’s everywhere
you want to be. Zoloft’s logo smiles from plastic pre-paid phone cards, coffee mugs, luggage tags, and
complimentary pens and pencils. A commercial during the World Series asserts the powers of Paxil to cure
social anxiety. A colorful tissue box in a physician’s office proclaims: “Sue’s playing with her kids again”
on one side and “Walter’s fishing again” on the other. The reason for the turnaround? “Just like normal—
Then there’s the National Depression Screening Day (NDSD). All across the country, hospitals,
mental health clinics, physicians offices, and even libraries, grocery stores, and shopping malls are helping
people suffering with depression, many of whom apparently do not even know they are suffering.
Sponsored by the American Psychiatric Association and National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) and
supported by mental health organizations and patient groups, this project has grown to include over 3000
sites. In 1998 it screened a record 90,000 people. Over the radio and on television, the message is the same:
depression, the silent killer, is a treatable, physical disease, like high blood pressure. “Help” is just a phone
At the screening sites, the message continues: mild forms of depression can be helped with
counseling; however, moderate or severe forms of the disease require medication. Never mind that no
depressed person we know of would ever describe his or her own suffering as “mild.” It’s like “I’m mildly
pregnant.” But, of course, that is precisely the point. Why? Because in spite of being jointly sponsored by
the American Psychiatric Association and the NIMH, NDSD is actually almost completely funded by drug
companies. In fact, six of the seven major funders are pharmaceutical companies. Kathleen Day reported in
her 1995 article “Depression Awareness—or a Prozac Pitch?” in the Washington Post, that Eli Lilly alone
provides 50% of the funding! The same article reported student and parent complaints that the project,
extended into schools, seemed little more than a plug for Prozac. Say no to drugs, but say yes to Prozac.
Perhaps Prozac needs a cool image for maximum impact—perhaps something like Joe Camel.
Primary care physicians, who write most prescriptions for antidepressants, are prime targets for
this national marketing extravaganza disguised as a public awareness project. For example, after a recent
Depression Day, the managed mental health care firm, Pacificare, reported that the typical physician in its
plan, with the help of the quick and dirty questionnaire administered to Shelley, now identifies two to four
depressed patients a day and prescribes medication. The message is well-crafted, and it works. If Prozac,
Paxil and Zoloft were books, they would be runaway best sellers: $7 billion was spent on them last year.
Finally, consider the recent White House Conference on Mental Health. Leading luminaries in the
field of medicine gathered together to discuss “cutting edge” discoveries in the treatment of emotional
problems. As columnist Adrianna Huffington later pointed out in a June 1999 article (Adrianna Online),
however, the whole affair was, “mainly a cheerleading session for drug manufacturers,” with the plenary
sessions looking like “infomercials.” Adrianna’s take on the conference message was that, contrary to the
first lady’s suggestion, it doesn’t take a village to raise a child, just a pill. Adrianna’s conclusion
notwithstanding, this “historic” conference provides commentary on the pervasiveness of the bad chemicals
on the brain theory of human suffering and the belief in the myth of the magic pill.
This widespread popularity suggests that antidepressants, especially SSRIs, are not merely one
good therapeutic tool among many, but miracle drugs, manna from the gods, universal panaceas. They have
no serious side effects, so the conventional wisdom goes. They supposedly work for everybody, and far
faster and more effectively than therapy for serious depression, making them the obvious treatment of first
resort. They help a profoundly depressed sufferer get out of bed and walk and have “proved”, so the
popular wisdom goes, that the causes of depression and other forms of human suffering lie not in our stars
The message behind the miracle play is demoralizing and disempowering to both clients and
therapists--to say nothing of its being utterly unrealistic. Yet professional associations representing
therapists seem to have believed the drug companies’ publicity and accepted their second class status,
assuming the primacy of pharmaceuticals is based not on great marketing, but good science. They have
become acolytes, worshiping before the altar on which the magic pill is displayed. For example, having
apparently resigned themselves to a “if you can’t beat psychiatry, then join them” philosophy, the
American Psychological Association is fighting for prescription privileges for psychologists. At the same
time, the American Association for Marriage and Family Therapy (AAMFT) is funding their campaign for
wider recognition as a legitimate provider organization by seeking grants from various pharmaceutical
companies. AAMFT recently joined Glaxo-Wellcome (a drug company) to produce a brochure called
“Intimacy and Depression: The Silent Epidemic.” Drugs are spotlighted as the treatment of choice for
depression. Curiously, family therapy is never mentioned, and therapy of any kind becomes a poor second
cousin to medication--“antidepressants are usually effective, [but] psychotherapy can also be useful,” the
brochure condescendingly points out. It also laments about the many sexual side effects of antidepressants,
but suggests that other choices exist without sexual side effects. Luckily, Glaxo-Wellcome makes
Wellbutrin, an antidepressant whose major marketing distinction is its lack of sexual side effects.
Wellbutrin is not mentioned in the brochure, allowing the AAMFT to maintain its posture of never
endorsing products, but a veiled and ghostly endorsement nonetheless hovers around the entire production,
whatever the high-minded denials. Family therapists and other non-medical therapists can either accept a
second class status, or face daunting odds in protesting the erosion of valued traditions in their professional
While most people first became aware of our concerns about medication when we protested the
AAMFT and Glaxo-Wellcome relationship, we actually began questioning the veracity of the magic pill
many years ago. While a graduate student, Barry worked in a residential treatment center for troubled
adolescents. When the psychiatrist was on vacation, 16 year old Ann was admitted and assigned to Barry.
Ann was like many of the kids, abused in all imaginable ways, drop kicked from one foster home to
another, attempted suicide on a number of occasions, and been in numerous hospitals and runaway shelters.
In spite of all that, Ann was a pure delight—creative, funny, and hopeful for a better future. Therapy went
great, Barry and Ann hit it off famously, and Ann settled in and attended high school for the first time in
Three weeks later the psychiatrist returned. Though Ann was adamantly opposed to medication—
she said she had been down that path already--the doctor ordered an antidepressant and lithium. Barry
protested citing evidence of how well Ann was doing, but to no avail—he was only a mental health grunt
and a student to boot. Ann soon ran away and went on a three-day binge of alcohol and drugs. A carload of
older men who picked her up while hitchhiking ended the ride with a gang rape. Adding insult to injury,
Ann was forcefully injected with an antipsychotic when the police brought her back to the center. When
Ann described this experience to Barry, she saw the horror on his face and reassured him that she had
suffered far worse indignities than being forcefully tranquilized. It was little solace for either though.
When Ann persisted in her ardent protests of the drugs, Barry encouraged her to talk to the center
director. Rather than listening, however, the director defended the psychiatrist. Later on, the director
admonished Barry for putting ideas into Ann’s head and told him to “drop it.” Instead, Barry spent days
researching the literature. What he found surprised him. In contrast to what most clients were told, little
was actually known about how psychotropic drugs actually worked. There were drugs like cocaine, for
example, which blocked the reuptake of the brain chemicals believed critical to depression in exactly the
same way as antidepressants but did not have any “therapeutic effect.” Furthermore, while increases in
these “critical” neurotransmitters resulting from antidepressants were actually present within hours of the
first dose they did not result in any therapeutic benefit for 4 to 6 weeks! Moreover, there was no empirical
support for prescribing these drugs to children--let alone multiple drugs.
Finally, Barry was shocked to find that the actual effectiveness of the drugs was suspect. He found
a 1974 review of 91 studies that reported that tricyclic antidepressants had no better effect than a sugar pill
in nearly one third of the published reports. Though largely overlooked, this finding is particularly
noteworthy because participants who showed rapid improvement to the fake pill (called, “place
bo responders”) were eliminated from these studies! Furthermore, as research with negative
results is less likely to be published, one can safely assume that the extent of the placebo response rate was
considerably underestimated in this review. Simply put, Barry had unexpectedly discovered that the
emperor had no clothes. What did Barry get when he confronted the psychiatrist with these facts? He got
fired. Ann survived as usual, resisting when she could, and unfortunately viewed this experience as just
another cog in her wheel of abuse from her “helpers.” Barry left demoralized but determined never to be in
the dark again, complicit by virtue of ignorance.
Despite the saturation of the message and the bottomless pit of money that supports it, it cannot
succeed without our cooperation. They need talk therapists to promote their message. Increasing their
market share requires that everyone come to believe that chemicals are both the cause and the cure to
emotional difficulties. As Barry discovered, raising a voice of concern is risky. As the old saying goes,
there’s not much difference between holding up your head and sticking your neck out. However, continuing
to acquiesce to the biological perspective and prostituting ourselves for whatever is leftover after the “real
doctors” have finished eating will only insure our permanent placement at the “kiddies” table. This doesn’t
mean simply closing our mouths and refusing to take the medicine we’re being told is good for us. Rather,
it means looking at what the research literature really says and then using that information to help clients
navigate the many choices available to them. In particular, in order to stay the rising tide of biological
fundamentalism, clinicians need to be aware of several serious shortcomings in the research.
To begin with, SSRIs do not work for everybody, not even for those who are desperate to believe
in them. Just consult The Physician’s Desk Reference. It reports that adverse reactions cause 15-16% of
people to discontinue treatment and that little is known about their effectiveness or consequences beyond
12 weeks of use. A 1999 report issued by the Agency for Health Care Policy and Research (AHCRP) found
that in spite of being marketed as having “fewer side effects,” those actually taking the new and improved
drugs didn’t think so. In fact, they were just as likely to drop out of research studies because of side effects
as those who took the older tricyclic drugs. Patients on SSRIs are more likely to complain of diarrhea,
nausea, insomnia, agitation, headache, and sexual problems. The tricyclic antidepressants are more likely to
cause dry mouth, constipation, dizziness, blurred vision, tremors, and adverse cardiovascular effects. Pick
Unfortunately, dropping out of a research study is among the least problematic side effects of
these pharmaceuticals. Adverse drug reactions are in fact the third leading cause of death in the United
States! In a 1995 study published in the prestigious Archives of Internal Medicine, pharmacists Lyle
Bootman and Jeffrey Johnson investigated 3.1 million hospital admissions and estimated that 200,000
people die per year from drug complications. This figure is equivalent to a 757 packed full of passengers
crashing every eight hours every day of the year! The same study set the cost of such “reactions” at $77
The adverse effects getting the most coverage lately, especially since the Columbine shootings and
the death of Phil Hartman, is the increased chance of violence. According to psychiatrist outcast Peter
Breggin, in his 1999 book Your Drug May Be Your Problem, “there is substantial evidence that…SSRI’s
can cause or exacerbate depression, suicide, paranoia and violence.” Psychologist Ann Blake Tracy
investigated 32 murder/suicides in her book Prozac: Panacea or Pandora? She found that 24 of these 32
cases were taking SSRI’s. Is this just the raving of lunatics? Yes according to spokespersons like Frederick
Goodwin, former director of the NIMH, who boldly asserts that the question of psychotropic drug safety
and effectiveness “has long been settled by a mass of scientific evidence and by the testimonies of hundreds
of thousands of patients, their families, and caregivers.” When this “mass of scientific evidence” is
considered, however, the supposed superiority of biological intervention is exposed as a house of cards
First, consider the report of the AHCPR, which reviewed more than 300 randomized trials of the
SSRIs for depression. The report concluded that the SSRI’s were no more effective in treating depression
than the older and much less costly tricyclic antidepressants. Moreover, in contrast to the 75-80% success
rates frequently touted in promotional literature by drug companies, the AHCRP reported a much more
modest 50% response rate to the drugs. In other words, only half of those given an antidepressant actually
experienced some benefit. While at first glance this figure may still seem impressive, the researchers found
that 32% of people in the studies they reviewed responded just as well to an inert, inactive placebo! This
means that the newer anti-depressants only outperformed sugar pills by 18%--a finding hardly worth
writing home about. Responding to similar data published by AHCPR in 1993, psychiatrist Walter Brown
ironically stated, “That’s not an astonishing effect,” and provocatively proposed that placebo should be the
first line of treatment with depression.
Recall the stunning changes in D in the 60 minutes broadcast. Her changes occurred in just 2
weeks although prevailing guidelines for Prozac are 4-6 weeks. Which did she respond to? The active drug
or placebo factors? Blinded by expensive marketing, Americans have been led to believe that the virtually
universal effectiveness of antidepressants is a matter of scientific record, conclusively demonstrated in
strict, controlled, double-blind, placebo studies--the gold standard in medical research. But, in fact, the
exaggerated claims from mediocre results are not based on empirical proof but on relatively flimsy data and
In their provocative tour de force, From Placebo to Panacea, Professors Roger Greenberg and the
late Seymour Fisher demonstrate that the validity of controlled studies, in which a placebo is compared to
the “real” drug, depends upon the participants and the raters who measure the effects not knowing who is
getting the real drug and who is getting the placebo. They point out, however, that the use of inert sugar
pills as the placebo in the vast majority of drug studies actually makes it possible for everyone involved to
tell who is taking the real drug. Simply put, those taking the active medication will be more likely to
experience the standard side effects--dry mouth, weight loss or gain, dizziness, headache, constipation,
nausea, insomnia, etc.--clear signals that they are taking a powerful drug--while those taking the sugar pill
will not. As a result, the “double-blind” study is immediately “unblinded.”—a fact which seriously
compromises any conclusions that can be drawn.
Paradoxically, side effects by themselves likely account for the effect seen in antidepressant
studies. A review that examined 13 (all available at the time) studies on Prozac by Roger Greenberg and his
associates in a 1994 issue of the Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease found that side effects were
themselves positively correlated with improvement. They reported that the greater the experience of side
effects, the better the outcome was judged to be by both patient and clinician. A meta-analytic review of
drug treatments for obsessive compulsive disorder similarly found judgements of therapeutic benefit rose as
the experience of side effects increased. These studies suggest that a sudden nudge to clients’ physical
perceptions seemed to jump-start their own capacity for emotional regeneration.
Psychologists and respected scientists Irving Kirsch and Guy Sapirstein make a persuasive case that
antidepressants may have no effect on depression other than that produced by the perception of side effects
and the power of placebo. Their meta-analytic review of 19 studies involving 2318 patients showed that the
75% of the beneficial effect of antidepressants can be ascribed to the placebo effect. The remaining 25% of
the positive effect of antidepressant is attributable to the side effects. This review demonstrates that
antidepressants are equivalent to credible, but non antidepressant drugs; in other words, when an active
placebo is used (one that mimics the side effects of the real drug), the advantage for the antidepressant
disappears--there is no difference in discernible effect between the placebo and the drug being tested.
Several other recent meta-analytic studies from independent research groups have validated the finding that
placebo accounts for most of the antidepressant effect.
Finally, drug studies often look better than they are because they rate improvement by looking to
clinicians’ perceptions, not clients’. They usually rely on clinician-rated measures of depression (the
Hamilton Depression Rating Scale or the Global Assessment Scale, for example) rather than client-rated
measures (like the Beck Depression Inventory or the Lambert and Burlingame Outcome Questionnaire).
But clinicians and clients differ substantially in their reading of how much improvement in emotional well
being the drugs bring about. In 1986, outcome researcher Michael Lambert and colleagues discovered in
their meta-analysis of antidepressant studies that clients reported significantly less improvement on drugs
than did their therapists. Six years later, in 1992, Greenberg and colleagues published another more
extensive meta-analysis of 22 antidepressant studies involving 2230 patients--and compared the effects of a
placebo with both “old” (Elavil, for example) and “new” (Prozac) antidepressants. They found that [both
old and new] antidepressants showed an advantage [about 18%] over the placebo on clinician-rated
measures, but none on client-rated measures.
In short, when clients rate their own responses, they usually experience no improvement on
antidepressants beyond what can be attributed to hope and expectation. If clients don’t feel “better” after
taking medications, how meaningful is any “improvement” their therapists think they see? As a final
example of the exaggeration of meager findings, consider a study recently showcased as “the one” that finally
demonstrates the benefit of Prozac with children. Set aside the problem of the inert placebo and compromise
of the double blind. This study showed no differences between the Prozac and placebo groups on all measures,
both clinician and client rated, (totaling five) except one. One clinician rated measure of improvement showed
superior performance of the Prozac over the placebo condition at the end of the study. The conclusion
“Fluoxetine was superior to placebo in the acute phase treatment of major depressive disorder in
child and adolescent outpatients with severe, persistent depression.”
Antidepressants are heavily marketed as more effective than therapy for severe depression, and as
the pharmaceutical bubble continues to swell, managed care plans have inexorably pruned therapy to a bare
minimum in favor of medications. But research has for years demonstrated that drugs are no more effective
than therapy--and there is growing evidence that they may even be less effective. As just one example of
such research, consider the largest and most methodologically sound study conducted to date comparing
psychotherapy with drug treatment: The Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Project or
TDCRP, led by psychologist Irene Elkin.This 1989 NIMH project, which involved psychiatrists and
psychologists in multiple cities, randomly assigned 250 participants to four groups: Aaron Beck’s cognitive
therapy, Gerald Klerman and Myrna Weissman’s interpersonal therapy, antidepressant treatment, and
finally placebo. Overall, the four treatments--including the placebo--worked with about the same
Since the study was first published, there is now research evidence that changes brought about by
therapy are more likely to persist over time. In 1992, researcher Tracie Shea and colleagues published an
18-month follow-up study of clients in the original 1989 NIMH multi-site project. The psychotherapies
outperformed the medications and placebo on almost every outcome measure. More therapy clients than
drug clients recovered without a subsequent major depressive relapse, while those receiving the
antidepressants sought treatment more often during the follow-up period, showed a higher probability of
relapse, and experienced fewer weeks of minimal or no symptoms than either the two therapy groups or the
Over the decades, generations of therapists have come to suspect it isn’t so much what they do--
what theory, what model, what technique or even what medication--that helps people, but who they are and
who their clients are, as well as the idiosyncratic personal fit between themselves and the people who come
to see them. Now, there is a growing body of solid evidence for this widespread intuitive wisdom. A study
conducted by Sidney Blatt and colleagues based on the same massive data pool comprising the 1989 NIMH
project, reinforced evidence that has been emerging in other studies for years: the difference in outcome
was related more to differences among clients and therapists than to treatment methods. Blatt found,
however, that some therapists were more effective than others. Who were they? The researchers learned
that the clinicians most successful in treating depression were more likely to use psychotherapy alone--they
rarely used medications at all. “More effective therapists have a psychological rather than a biological
orientation in their treatment approach,” Blatt concluded.
But wouldn’t the best of all possible worlds be one in which medications were combined with therapy,
for a kind of double whammy treatment effect? This idea that both together must be better than either one
alone for treating depression has become the newest orthodoxy among many professional groups. In fact,
this sensible-sounding “compromise” solution actually promotes the use of medications, by implicitly
suggesting that virtually anybody who enters therapy for any reason could usefully take them, and many
managed care funded practices now routinely require all therapy clients to undergo medical evaluations as a
prerequisite to treatment. And yet, there is little evidence in favor of the two-is-better-than-one approach. In
1998, Larry Beutler, researcher and senior editor of The Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
challenged anyone to find current scientific literature supporting this now-conventional belief. No one can.
Consider a meta-analytic study by Yale psychiatrist Bruce Wexler who concluded his review of seven well-
controlled studies of 513 patients with this simple comparison: out of 100 patients with major depression,
29 would recover if given drugs alone, 47 would recover if given therapy alone, and 47 would recover if
given combined treatment. On the other hand, drop out or poor response can be expected in 52 drug
patients, 30 therapy patients, and 34 combined patients. Further, a 1995 Consumer Reports study concurs
that medication plus psychotherapy contributed no more benefits that psychotherapy alone. These findings
suggest that therapy alone should usually be the initial plan rather than expose clients to unnecessary costs and
Reality check: The preponderance of scientific evidence shows that therapy is as effective or more
effective than medications in the treatment of depression, even if severe, especially when client-rated
measures and long-term follow-up are considered. In all of the healing arts, there is no single explanation
nor simple, infallible remedy for any of the problems that beset humankind. And yet, the growing focus on
biological determinism in mental health with the accompanying pharmaceutical hard-sell suggests not only
that there are always solely biological explanations, but perfect, fail-safe biological solutions as well--
simple pills that mark fini to everything from mild depression and nervous tension to panic attacks and
bipolar disorder, to full-blown psychosis and schizophrenia. How did this medically anomalous, weirdly
simplistic point of view come about? If the science behind the alleged superiority of psychotropic drugs is
so lacking, how did medications come to hold almost unchallenged sway over both public and professional
In the days of the Watergate investigation, the chief informant known as “Deep Throat” advised
investigative reporters to “follow the money” to discover the source of illegal behaviors, which eventually
led them to the president of the United States. Something like the same advice helps explain why
psychotropic medications have permeated every aspect of our culture. Follow the money, and you will
begin to understand the logic behind the growth of the pharmaceutical behemoth. Last year, a piece of
investigative journalism in The Wall Street Journal, for example, reported that 96 percent of the research
studies of a drug funded by its manufacturers turn out favorable results, while only 37 percent of such drug
studies not funded by the manufacturer find in favor of the new drug. Like a flower opening itself to the
sun, the research results tend to be skewed in the direction of the money source. Similarly, a study just
published in October, 1999 in The Journal of the American Medical Association(JAMA) by Mark
Friedberg concluded that drug company sponsorship is associated with reduced likelihood of reporting
unfavorable results. In a scientific version of the piper calling the tune, the drug company paying for the
research tends to get the kind of research its leaders want.
Given this bias, it is even more distressing to consider how many pro drug articles are published
that fail to disclose ties to drug companies. The American Medical Association provides ethical prohibitions
against physicians accepting payments, funding, reimbursements, or inducements of any kind without
disclosing their relationships to drug companies. But it happens anyway, all too often. A recent investigation
by Terence Monmaney of the Los Angeles Times found that the renowned New England Journal of Medicine published articles by researchers with drug company ties who did not identify potential conflicts
of interest. In an analysis of 36 articles since 1997, eight articles were found with undisclosed financial
links to drug companies that marketed treatments evaluated in the articles. Another recent example is
provided by an article in the JAMA that reported the sorry state of affairs of American sexuality, saying that as
many as 43% suffer from sexual dysfunction. JAMA belatedly disclosed that the authors of the study were
paid consultants to Pfizer, makers of Viagra, and may possibly had a conflict of interest in pronouncing the
nations libido in such a dysfunctional condition.
While the AMA takes conflicts of interest seriously, it cannot apparently keep up with all the
violations. With so much drug company funding, it is impossible to police. In addition, there is a major
loophole in the guidelines. While disclosure is mandated in medical journals, it is not required in any
publications that go directly to the general public. It is almost like the AMA is saying that is important for
doctors to know about potential breaches in objectivity but not the general public. A physician can write an
article in a magazine with widespread circulation or author a book that sells millions and never have to
breathe a word about their affiliations. Add to this disclosure deficit the many TV appearances made that
implicitly support drug company interests.
Just as bad money drives out good, heavy marketing seems to blunt or even nullify the effects of
good, but negative research when it does occur. Consumer Reports estimated in a 1992 issue that the 65
billion-dollar drug industry spends 5 billion dollars a year on promotion and publicity for its products--the
“educational” and “public service” efforts and multimedia advertising blitz already discussed, for example.
Psychotherapy cannot begin to compete with the billion dollar drug industry when it comes to promoting the
value of therapy even though the data are clear: psychotherapy is hands down as good or better when head-to-
head comparisons with medication are made.
Psychiatrist gadflies Peter Breggin and Loren Mosher have documented the powerful influence of
drug company money on continuing education and psychiatric journals. Mosher, in a 1999 Psychology Today article, estimates that drug companies pay an average of $10,000 per physician, per year, on
“education.” Fully, 30% of the American Psychiatric Association budget is underwritten by drug
advertising and pharmaceutical companies substantially support psychiatric conferences through displays
and unrestricted grants. How does all this money impact the psychiatric profession? As Abraham Lincoln
once said, “Moral principle is a looser bond than pecuniary interest.”
Mosher calls the relationship between drug companies and psychiatry an “unholy alliance” that “is
dangerous because researchers and psychiatrists…remain biased in favor of drug cures, downplay side
effects and seldom try other types of intervention.” It is understandable that biological psychiatry is now
embraced almost exclusively in medical schools and residency training programs. “Biochemical
imbalance” is the battle cry of the profession. In interviews with the media, psychiatrists often resemble the
brainwashed soldiers in the Manchurian Candidate (starring Frank Sinatra), robotically espousing the
ubiquitous biochemical imbalance as the cause for a plethora of problems.
And therapists of all stripes are joining them as drug companies expand their influence—and the
potential conflicts of interest--to other mental health professions. For example, with the help of an
unrestricted grant from GlaxoWellcome, the 1998 conference of the AAMFT highlighted the Intimacy and
Depression campaign at its opening session—a session generally reserved for one of the real movers of the
family therapy field. Instead, without disclosing GlaxoWellcome’s involvement, the session, using “Party
of Five” clips and an Oprah style format presented an hour and a half commercial for Wellbutrin without,
of course, ever mentioning the drug specifically. The tragedy of the sexual side effects of antidepressants
was repeated 11 times and 6 times the point was pummeled into the audience of 2000 therapists that
treating depression need not include depriving clients of a sex life. Therapists were encouraged to inquire
about side effects and inform clients that alternatives exist to help them.
Two of the presenters are affiliated with GlaxoWellcome. The psychiatrist who drove home the point
about sexual side effects of antidepressants is on Glaxo's advisory board, speaker's bureau, and has had her
research funded by them. She, with the psychologist on the panel, wrote GlaxoWellcome's own brochure about
the IDC. By depriving the audience of the consideration of any possible conflicts of interest, AAMFT violated
the standards of the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education (ACCME) in two ways. AAMFT
did not acknowledge Glaxo’s support in the conference brochure (Standard 5c) and did not disclose the faculty
With all this largesse and publicity raining benevolently down, is it any wonder that people and
therapists tend to become hypnotically fixated on the brouhaha about a “revolution” in
psychopharmaceuticals and overlook the boring fine print of the drug studies with their more negative
implications? Importantly, the fact that drugs do not live up to their miracle status does not discredit those
that have been helped. Medication has its place—if only it would stay there! To give the devil his due, we
believe that antidepressants can be very helpful at times--especially for those who believe in them. Because
they’ve gotten good press, they can positively harness the placebo effect, reinforcing Sir William Osler’s
dictum that “One should treat as many patients as possible with a new drug while it still has the power to
heal.” They sometimes do help free people from paralyzing self-doubt, obsessions and bulimic cravings--
people who have conscientiously tried therapy with no results. Because they’re widely touted as simple and
effective, they encourage people who might otherwise be too ashamed and reluctant, to talk to another
human being about their unhappiness and pain. Because they offer concrete, immediate action, they hearten
people with the thought that at least someone is doing something to help them. Finally, some people find it
easier to begin contemplating their distress when they can attribute it to their biochemistry rather than to
circumstances in their personal lives. So they should not be banished from the sanctuary of psychotherapy;
rather, therapists and consumers should get a grip, stop kowtowing to their supposedly superior powers,
and think of them as one valid choice among many--and certainly not as the treatment of first resort.
But where does this sobering evidence about psychiatric drugs leave us when we are face to face
with smothering despair? Consider Alina. At the first session, her therapist listened with growing concern
as Alina talked about her fourth attempt to live apart from her abusive husband—the sense of having failed,
failed her husband, her marriage, her children, and herself. She spoke of her financial desperation, the
humiliation of her new job with a boss who berated her Spanish accent in front of customers, and her guilt
at not being able to make a better life, pick a better partner, or make a bad marriage better. “I can hardly get
up in the morning. It’s just no use. I’m no good to anyone. I really have nothing to live for, to look forward
to. If I had the guts, I’d just crash my car into a tree and be done with it.” Her tears, her anguish, her despair
were so palpable during the session that her therapist found herself having to fight her own feelings
Such riveting situations are, if not routine, expected fare in our line of business. It is the “burned
out” therapist, indeed, who cannot resonate with the suffering and seemingly inescapable dilemmas our
clients present. And it is this resonance that permits the connection and, therefore, the possibility that we
can be part of some kind of change. It is also this resonance that makes us vulnerable to finding ourselves,
like our clients, in the land of no alternatives. And when, in that place, a voice appears speaking of the
miraculous wonders of a biological cure, the superiority of “modern science” over primitive “talk” or “self-
help,” the sky brightens and a gleam of hope shatters the darkness. With no other equally attractive,
powerful voice around, it’s not difficult to see that, when facing the hard issues (like life or death),
therapists with a heart naturally, inevitably reach for something they believe can give their clients (and
Our culture, our “mental health” mythology, speaks the following: “It’s fine to go to self-help
groups, talk to a therapist, try yoga, or take St. John’s wort when you’re just having a down time—the
“blues.” We’re talking about a different animal; we’re talking about the can’t eat, can’t sleep, can’t not
sleep, can’t think, can’t work, can’t love kind of debilitation that threatens one’s livelihood, indeed, one’s
very life. We’re talking major depression—MD.” How has such a climate flourished, where one option
shines out above all others in such a compelling and irresistible way, virtually assuring it’s prominence in
the lives of people in severe distress? How is it that forty years of research that confirms the preeminence
of client resourcefulness, resiliency, and the healing capacity of the therapeutic relationship are dwarfed by
a pill? How has it come to be that therapists, when put to the test, so readily abandon their beliefs in hard-
won, professional skills and instincts in favor of a medical solution?
Just to set the record straight, these are the very situations we most challenge therapists and
clients to re-examine and to question the knee-jerk reaction to seek prescription. We challenge the belief
that such depressive conditions, while undoubtedly fraught with pain, despair, and fear, universally require
medication. In fact, we challenge therapists and clients to consider personal and social options on at least an
equal footing—and to rebuild a faith in themselves and the inevitability of change.
Sitting there, facing Alina, the therapist found herself wondering about the best way to be of help.
She began to think about medication, and the implications of the fact that 70% of all antidepressants are
prescribed to women. Would medication augment Alina’s sense of failure or provide the boost she needed
to find hope and pleasure in her life once again? One path was predictable—referral to someone who could
prescribe would inevitably focus on Alina’s mental state, her fragility, and potential suicide. Other paths
were far more uncertain. As the therapist struggled with the uncertainty, Alina talked about her past
relationships, sharing some sad, and even some humorous, memories. As the conversation unfolded, Alina
mentioned that she didn’t like pills, and wanted “to do it on her own.” Knowing that various plans,
including medication, could be introduced at any time, the therapist trusted in Alina’s direction and in the
power of two people inhabiting an intimate moment of the tragedies and triumphs of Alina’s life. It seemed
natural, at the end of the visit, for the therapist to comment on what Alina had done to extricate herself and
her children in the past from unbearable situations. Alina ended the discussion by emphatically stating that
she had no plans to hurt herself. Three weeks later, after weekly meetings, the worst seemed to be over.
Alina had managed to hang in with her new job and was quietly putting money away as her “escape plan”
for an independent future. The therapist and Alina had weathered the storm together, with Alina, not
medication (or the therapist), at the helm.
To be trigger happy to bring medication into the discussion automatically in “scary” sessions is to
be under the influence of bad science and great marketing. When clients are stuck or desperate, the
medication solution is easy to whip up—like ready-made dinners, it takes the work and anxiety out of
“What’s for supper?” However, consider also that in most therapy, therapists hold more power than clients.
Consequently, therapist suggestions, for better or worse, immediately garner a particular status in clients’
minds. The introduction of medication at a particular point in the therapy conversation carries with it
numerous messages, none of which is “Here’s just one of many options to consider.” Instead, the uninvited
introduction of medication into the therapy conversation, even when framed as an option, is likely to
communicate “Your problem is so severe, we have to look at something other than what we are doing here,
or something other than what you can do to deal with it;” or “Medication is the best thing for what you are
telling me.” These messages serve to abort the natural search for answers that is the heart of change.
In a moment of crisis, of wanting to help or seeking help, if the option to try something different is
not as least as attractive, doable, and potentially effective as the medical option, the magic pill will win.
What is required is a shift, or, more likely, a reconnection with what therapists know and have experienced
over and over, both in their clients and in themselves—that most people can and will develop solutions to
even the most daunting dilemmas given support and encouragement, that the impetus to health has many
avenues and sometimes takes unorthodox routes, and that change will and does occur naturally and
universally. This is not a “just snap out of it” or “grin and bear it” approach to either “character weakness”
or moodiness. It is an effective, all-out assault on the sometimes terrifying and almost always destructive
experience of depression. At its core is a faith in change and the human tendency to find a way even out of
We privilege this stance as way to “level the playing field,” to compete with the powerful and
boisterous medical ideologies promoted by profiteers and championed by factions within our own
professions. When we can hang onto these beliefs in our hearts, emboldened by personal, anecdotal, and
empirical evidence, we, and our clients, can hear, loud and clear, other possibilities in times of crisis where
once there was only the seductive chant to medicate. Recall, once again the NIMH Treatment of
Depression Collaborative Research Project found that clinical improvement was unrelated to the type of
treatment received (e.g., psychotherapy, drug treatment). Researcher Janice Krupnick and colleagues, using
the same data (reported in The Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology), have shown that the quality
and strength of the therapeutic relationship was the primary determinant of successful outcome across
treatments--including medication! The type of treatment administered didn’t matter. The type of
relationship formed mattered most. Indeed, the massive size of the NIMH study means that the best, most
empirically supported treatment for depression is a good relationship with a therapist.
For most of the history of the field, therapists have been trained and research conducted “as if”
treatment models and their associated techniques explained and caused change. Like the anesthetic before
surgery, “building an alliance” or “establishing rapport” has routinely been thought of as the procedure
therapists must do prior to the “real” treatment (e.g., confronting dysfunctional thinking, prescribing drugs,
etc.). In contrast to common perception, the therapeutic relationship is not another vague, unquantifiable,
“feel good” technique from the field of therapy. Neither is it the latest in a long line of miraculous
technique to be hyped on the lecture circuit. Rather, a virtual mountain of studies conducted over the last 40
years consistently find that therapies in which the client’s goals, ideas about the problem and change
process, and perceptions of a helpful therapeutic interaction are the most successful. Note the emphasis on
Here is where we differ than those that would apply the aggregate data about drugs and
psychotherapy without considering the client’s own views of what could be helpful. It is true that the data
suggest that psychotherapy should be the first line of treatment for people with experiences of depression,
then if change is not forthcoming, medication can be considered. However, such an assumption does not
integrate the unique aspects of what our work entails, nor does it include the most potent contributors to the
change process in the decision-making process—our clients. Listening to and exploring their stories,
experiences, and interpretations of the problem and the change process, what we have come to call the
client’s theory of change, over time, evolves to an approach that is tailored to the unique qualities of the
individual client. . In short, treatment is client-directed. Depending on the client’s views of what effectively
produces change, this could include anything from physical exercise and dietary changes to assertiveness
training, cognitive-behavioral therapy, volunteering, St. John’s Wort, restructuring family hierarchies or
learning how to get along better with others--all of which have been shown to sometimes have a positive
impact on depression We would, for example, never stand in the way of a client considering medication if
they believed their problems were of biological origin and thought the drugs might be helpful. It is up to
therapists to privilege clients’ wishes in the therapy conversation, including their trains of thought, their
brainstorming, and their talk. When clients put medication on the table, then therapists can naturally help
them explore it as an option. When clients believe medication will help, feel more hopeful at the possibility
of trying medication, and are “in the driver’s seat” in making an informed choice (including information
about side effects, length of treatment, and possibilities of relapse), then medication can be beneficial. To
follow the client’s lead is to maximize client participation, strengthen the therapeutic bond, and enhance
In a country that has come to expect, even demand miracles from the pharmaceutical companies, it
is little wonder that the chronic problems of drug therapy and the excesses of corporate marketing have
been largely ignored. We hope against hope that some pill, some simple and painless solution, will be the
cure-all for our emotional and familial woes. Finally realizing that psychiatric drug therapy is a profit-
driven industry, built on a flimsy science, may be the bad tasting medicine we've needed. Although it may
be hard to swallow, it is time for therapists to learn the data, reinvigorate their belief in therapy, and offer
clients real choices for addressing their concerns. Ultimately, therapists and consumers need to just sit
back, take a deep breath, and accept the truth about depression and other human travails: there is no better
medicine than enlisting your own strengths in a good therapeutic relationship.
This article is adapted from Barry Duncan and Scott Miller’s latest book The Heroic Client: Doing Client Directed Outcome Informed Therapy. Barry and Scott are co-founders of the Institute for the Study of
Therapeutic Change (ISTC) and co-authors of several books including The Heart and Soul of Change,Escape from Babel, and Psychotherapy with “Impossible” Cases. Barry is an Associate Professor in the
School of Social and Systemic Studies at Nova Southeastern University (NSU) and Scott is an
internationally acclaimed workshop presenter. Jacqueline Sparks collaborated on the Heroic Client project
and is a member of the ISTC team, as well as a Doctoral Candidate at NSU. The authors can be reached at
Plan B One Step Emergency Contraception (pill) What is Emergency Contraception? You can prevent pregnancy after intercourse by taking Plan B cannot prevent an ectopic pregnancy, which is Emergency Contraceptive pil s (also known as the when a fertilized egg attaches and grows outside the Morning After Pil or EC). The most common is uterus. This can be very dangerous and requires